·Investigation··Current Issue·  ·Achieve·  ·Search Articles·  ·Online Submission·  ·About IJO·

 

Evaluation of visual stress symptoms in age-matched dyslexic, Meares-Irlen syndrome and normal adults

 

Mana A. Alanazi1, Saud A. Alanazi1, Uchechukwu L. Osuagwu2

 

1Department of Optometry & Vision Science, King Saud University, Riyadh 11433, Saudi Arabia

2Ophthalmic and Visual Optics Laboratory Group, Institute of Health and Biomedical Innovation, Department of Optometry & Vision Sciences, Faculty of Health, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane 4059, Queensland, Australia

Correspondence to: Uchechukwu L. Osuagwu. Ophthalmic and Visual Optics Laboratory Group, Institute of Health and Biomedical Innovation, Department of Optometry & Vision Sciences, Faculty of Health, Queensland University of Technology, Q Block, Room 5WS36 60 Musk Avenue Kelvin Grove, Brisbane 4059, Queensland, Australia. u.osuagwu@qut.edu.au; uosuagoul@ksu.edu.sa

Received: 2015-04-18               Accepted: 2015-07-27

 

Abstract

AIM: To examine the prevalence of dyslexia and Meares-Irlen syndrome (MIS) among female students and determine their level of visual stress in comparison with normal subjects.

METHODS: A random sample of 450 female medical students of King Saud University Riyadh (age range, 18-30y) responded to a wide range of questions designed to accomplish the aims of this study. The detailed questionnaire consisted of 54 questions with 12 questions enquiring on ocular history and demography of participants while 42 questions were on visual symptoms. Items were categorized into critical and non-critical questions (CQ and NCQ) and were rated on four point Likert scale. Based on the responses obtained, the subjects were grouped into normal (control), dyslexic with or without MIS (Group 1) and subjects with MIS only (Group 2). Responses were analysed as averages and mean scores were calculated and compared between groups using one way analysis of variance to evaluate total visual stress score (TVSS=NCQ+CQ), critical and non-critical visual stress scores. The relationship between categorical variables such as age, handedness and condition were assessed with Chi-square test.

RESULTS: The completion rate was 97.6% and majority of the respondents (92%) were normal readers, 2% dyslexic and 6% had MIS. They were age-matched. More than half of the participants had visited an eye care practitioner in the last 2y. About 13% were recommended eye exercises and one participant experienced pattern glare. Hand preference was not associated with any condition but Group 1 subjects (3/9, 33%) were significantly more likely to be diagnosed of lazy eye than Group 2 (2/27, 7%) and control (27/414, 7%) subjects. The mean±SD of TVSS responses were 63±14 and it was 44±9 for CQ and 19±5 for NCQ. Responses from all three variables were normally distributed but the CQ responses were on the average more positive (82%) in Group 2 and less positive (46%) in Group 1 than control. With NCQ, the responses were equally less positive in Group 1 and 2 than control. Group 2 subjects showed significantly higher TVSS (P=0.002), NCQ (P=0.006) and CQ (P=0.008) visual stress scores than control but no difference between Group 1 and control subjects, was observed for all scores (P>0.05, for all comparisons).

CONCLUSION: The prevalence of dyslexia and MIS among Saudi female students was 2% and 6%, respectively. Critical questions performed best for assessing visual stress symptoms in dyslexic and MIS subjects. Generally, students with MIS were more sensitive to visual stress than normal students but dyslexics were more likely to present with a lazy eye than MIS and normal readers.

KEYWORDS: dyslexia; visual stress symptoms; critical questions; non-critical questions; Meares-Irlen syndrome; questionnaire

DOI:10.18240/ijo.2016.04.24

 

Citation: Alanazi MA, Alanazi SA, Osuagwu UL. Evaluation of visual stress symptoms in age-matched dyslexic, Meares-Irlen syndrome and normal adults. Int J Ophthalmol  2016;9(4):617-624

 

INTRODUCTION

Dyslexia describes a specific disability in learning to read despite normal intelligence and opportunity[1]. An unequivocal definition of dyslexia is difficult and can vary according to the discipline[2]. Among practitioners, the diagnosis of dyslexia depends on showing that an individual’s phonological, reading and spelling skills are well below what one would expect for their age and other aspects of basic intelligence[3-4]. However, dyslexia is much more than this. It is considered a neurological syndrome that involves much more than just reading and writing[5]. Dyslexic individuals may experience significant visual symptoms while reading. These symptoms may or may not be consistent with binocular visual anomalies but they may impede the desire to read and write, and perhaps ultimately handicap the learning process. The visual stress symptoms include perceptual distortion, asthenopia and headache, diplopia, blurred vision, visual confusion[2,6] (suggested to be as a result of poor binocular control causing reading and spelling errors)[7]. Although dyslexia is a lifelong condition[8-10], most dyslexic persons are able to adapt to their condition by developing coping strategies (e.g. try to avoid reading) that help them to overcome their reading difficulties.

There are no specific causes of dyslexia, but it is widely believed that about 10% to 15% percent of the population with dyslexia are due to a subtle neuro-developmental syndrome[11-13] or disorder thought to have a hereditary component[2,14]. Dyslexia with such hereditary, neurodevelopmental aspects has been described as developmental dyslexia. Individuals with developmental dyslexia present difficulties with decoding while comprehension is more intact[13].

The connection between visual processes and dyslexia has gained much publicity through the claims made by Irlen[15-17], who reported that many children with dyslexia suffer from a perceptual dysfunction she called Scotopic Sensitivity Syndrome. This condition is currently referred to as Meares-Irlen syndrome (MIS)[15,18]. MIS is often seen in children with dyslexia[19] but no clear definition has been given[3]. It is characterized by visual perceptual distortion such as illusion of images, colour and motion effects, and symptoms of visual stress such as asthenopia in the form of eye strain, tired eyes, headache and photophobia. Treatment of this condition involves the use of proprietary coloured[17] and precision[20] overlays.

The involvement of visual stress in MIS is quite obvious but the nature of the relationship between visual stress and dyslexia is still unclear. Among children and adults with and without dyslexia, evidence of a link between MIS and reading difficulties has been established[21-22]. Singleton and Henderson[21] investigated visual stress in dyslexic and non-dyslexic children using a bespoke, computerized visual stress screening test and found that the incidence of high level visual stress in dyslexic children was almost twice that in control children. They observed that dyslexic children who showed high visual stress scores reported more than 20% improvement in reading speed with colour overlay in Wilkins Rate Reading Test as compared with control children with high visual stress who showed between 5% and 10% improvement. For children with lower visual stress scores, those with dyslexia showed at least 5% improvement compared with <5% in controls when using coloured overlays[21]. This indicates that predominance or incidence of visual stress in dyslexics is higher than non-dyslexics.

Till date, many studies on dyslexia and its visual correlates have focused on children with only a few studies in adults[22-24] even with the identification of an urgent need to support dyslexic adults in higher education[25]. Also, because the symptoms of MIS and visual stress are non-specific, the need for a differentially diagnosis of the condition from other ophthalmology conditions, such as refractive error, binocular vision anomalies, and accommodative anomalies has been identified[26]. Adults particularly students are constantly involved in tasks that require a lot of reading and writing. These increases their risk of experiencing visual stress symptoms and in the presence of dyslexia, affects their workplace or studies[27]. In a literate society, it becomes more disadvantageous and is compounded by racism[5]. This study aims to examine the prevalence of dyslexia and MIS in a population of literate individuals[28] and whose field of study demand a high level of study time. The strict admission policy of the prestigious medical schools is sufficient guarantee of a generally high level of knowledge. Given that a common support strategy at undergraduate university level is the provision of extra time in examinations, this study will also subjectively evaluate visual stress levels that are inferred from reading associated symptoms scores among these students, determine the appropriateness of questions to be used in identifying higher levels of visual stress associated with reading, and compare visual stress levels among dyslexic, MIS and normal subjects in order to identify which group of adults are more sensitive to visual stress.

SUBJECTS AND METHODS

Subjects  A total of 450 female medical students of King Saud University aged between 18 to 30y were recruited over a 2-month period. All participants gave consent to participate in the study after the protocol has been fully explained. Ethical approval was obtained from King Saud University Ethical Committee and all procedures followed the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki and the participants were required to complete an extended set of survey questions. Contact details for investigators were available on the survey.

Questionnaire Design  A paper version of a validated questionnaire with a wide range of questions was randomly distributed among medical students of the university who were recruited by oral invitation from the investigators. The survey was designed to subjectively identify the type and the degree of symptoms associated with reading, and whether those symptoms hinder the learning process or are associated with a specific reading and/ or spelling difficulty. The detailed questionnaire consisted of 54 questions with 12 questions enquiring on participants’ ocular history and demography while 42 questions were on visual symptoms including: symptoms related to visual distortion at near and distance, binocular visual dysfunction and reading or writing errors experienced by the participants. Survey items were derived from previous studies on dyslexia, MIS and visual discomfort[21,26,29-31]. Students who responded positively to previous diagnoses of dyslexia and MIS were contacted at the end of data collection and the diagnosis was confirmed in all subjects.

Statistical Analysis  The results were entered into a Microsoft Excel spreadsheet (Microsoft, Redmond, WA, USA), overlaid to eliminate data errors and corrected. Data was analysed using SPSS version 16 (SPSS Inc. Chicago, Illinois, USA). Besides the questions on demography and ocular history which were close end questions (requiring yes/no answer), all other items in the questionnaire were rated on four point Likert scales from 1 (never) to 4 (always) and a neutral response was scored as zero.

Differences were considered significance at P<0.05. Analysis was to assess the construct validity by studying the correlation between the final subjects’ questionnaire scoring with the case condition for all subjects using Tukey’s post hoc analysis of one way ANOVA. Questionnaire items were divided into two classes of questions: critical and non-critical questions. Critical questions (CQ) were those that purely relate to the reading and/or writing errors and to the visual stress symptom associated with reading while the non-critical questions (NCQ) describe visual symptoms that are attributed to binocular vision dysfunctions rather than the underlying issue or topic. Total visual stress score (TVSS) is the sum of both CQ and NCQ scores. Based on the responses obtained from the categorical questions, the subjects were grouped into normal (control), dyslexic with or without MIS (Group 1) and subjects with MIS only (Group 2). Chi-square test were conducted with continuity correction to relate independent categorical items such as age, handedness, presence of refractive error, sensitivity to the light, history of eye exercises and sensitivity to pattern glare to case condition (dyslexia or MIS). The average responses for each group of participant were also presented graphically. To evaluate total, critical and non-critical visual stress scores amongst the subject, ANOVA and post hoc test were conducted.

RESULTS

Subjects’ Characteristics  Initially, 461 students agreed to participate. Six students withdrew from the study due to the length of the survey and 5 failed to respond to most of the questions. Only data from 450 participants (97.6%) who responded to majority of the survey questions were included in the analysis. Of these students, 92% (414/450) were normal subjects (age range, 18-30y) and 8% (36/450; age range 18-26y) had previously been diagnosed with reading difficulties. Of these subjects, 2% were dyslexic with or without MIS (9/450) and 6% (27/450) had MIS only and were using colour filters for reading. The subjects were matched in age. Percentage analysis of the responses elicited by questions on demography and characteristics of the subjects is shown in Table 1. More than half of the participants had visited an eye care practitioner in the last 2y and about 47% and 34% use prescription glasses and contact lenses, respectively. About 13% had been recommended eye exercises and only one participant responded positively to experiencing pattern glare.

Table 1 Result of analysis of responses on participants’ characteristics                                                                             n (%)

Responses of questions enquiring about oneself

Counts of responses

Total

Yes

No

Not response

Use prescription glasses

211 (46.9)

237 (52.7)

2 (0.4)

450 (100)

Use prescription contact lenses

153 (34.0)

296 (65.8)

1 (0.2)

450 (100)

Handedness

Right-handed

409 (90.9)

-

-

450 (100)

Left-handed

41 (9.1)

-

-

Visited optometrists in the last 2a

263 (58.4)

187 (41.6)

-

450 (100)

Previously diagnosed of lazy eye

32 (7.1)

418 (92.9)

-

450 (100)

Previously wore eye patch

56 (12.4)

394 (87.6)

-

450 (100)

Eye exercises previously recommended

57 (12.7)

393 (87.3)

-

450 (100)

Previously diagnosed with dyslexia

9 (2.0)

441 (98.0)

-

450 (100)

Sensitivity to light

3 (0.7)

447 (99.3)

-

450 (100)

Used coloured overlays/tinted glasses for reading

30 (6.7)

420 (93.3)

-

450 (100)

Sensitivity to pattern glare

1 (0.2)

449 (99.8)

-

450 (100)

 

Cross tabulation on Chi-square analysis showed no significant association (P>0.05) between all categorical measures (age, handedness, history of eye exercises etc.) and case condition (dyslexia with/without MIS or MIS) except previous diagnosis of lazy eye which was significant associated (Chi-square, χ2=9.6, P=0.008) with dyslexia (Group 1). The subjects in Group 1 (3/9, 33%) were more likely to receive a diagnosis of lazy eye than Group 2 (2/27, 7%) and control (27/414, 7%) subjects.

Result of Analysis on Responses from Critical and Non-critical Questions  The visual stress scores from all three variables were normally distributed. The mean±SD of responses for TVSS was 63±14 and it was 44±9 for CQ and 19±5 for NCQ. The average responses from CQ and NCQ are shown in Figures 1 and 2 respectively for control, Group 1 and Group 2 subjects. With respect to the use of CQ, the response was on the average more positive (82%) for group 2 subjects and less positive (46%) for Group 1 subjects compared with the response from the control. Group 2 subjects (i.e. MIS subjects who did not receive a diagnosis of dyslexia but used coloured filters while reading) scored the highest when CQ was used to assess visual stress followed by Group 1 (dyslexic with MIS or without MIS) and control subjects (Figure 3).

Mana A. Alanazi1

Figure 1 Analysis of average responses on visual stress symptoms using critical questions in dyslexics with or without Meares-Irlen syndrome MIS (Group 1) and Meares-Irlen syndrome subjects (Group 2).

Mana A. Alanazi2

Figure 2 Analysis of average responses on visual stress symptoms using non-critical questions (NCQ) in dyslexics with or without Meares-Irlen syndrome (Group 1) and Meares-Irlen syndrome subjects (Group 2).

Mana A. Alanazi3

Figure 3 Mean visual stress scores with 95% limits of confidence intervals (CI) for critical questions in control, dyslexic with or without MIS and MIS subjects  The subjects with MIS are those who did not receive a diagnosis of dyslexia but use coloured filters to help in reading; dyslexic without MIS are subjects who have been diagnosed as dyslexic but do not use coloured filters to help in reading while dyslexics with MIS are subjects who have been diagnosed as dyslexic and use coloured filters to help in reading.

 

Concerning NCQ visual stress measure, the responses were on average equally positive in Group 1 and 2 but more positive than control (Figure 2). On analysis, the mean score was highest for Group 1 followed by Group 2 and control (Figure 4) and the TVSS was highest for Group 2 subjects than Group 1 and control (Figure 5). Tukey’s homogenous test showed that the sample size in each group did not influence the results of statistical analysis when total TVSS (P=0.085), CQ (P=0.148), NCQ (P=0.080) were used as measure variables. Analysis using one way ANOVA showed no statistical significant differences in mean score of subjects within groups for TVSS, CQ and NCQ (P>0.05, for all) but between groups, the TVSS score (P=0.003), the CQs (P=0.012) and NCQs (P=0.002) visual stress scores were statistically significantly different.

Mana A. Alanazi4

Figure 4 Mean visual stress scores with 95% limits of CI for non-critical questions in control, dyslexic with or without MIS and MIS subjects. The subjects with MIS are those who did not receive a diagnosis of dyslexia but use coloured filters to help in reading; dyslexic without MIS are subjects who have been diagnosed as dyslexic but do not use coloured filters to help in reading while dyslexics with MIS are subjects who have been diagnosed as dyslexic and use coloured filters to help in reading.

Mana A. Alanazi5

Figure 5 Mean visual stress scores with 95% limits of CI for TVSS in control, dyslexic with or without MIS and MIS subjects. The subjects with MIS are those who did not receive a diagnosis of dyslexia but use coloured filters to help in reading; dyslexic without MIS are subjects who have been diagnosed as dyslexic but do not use coloured filters to help in reading while dyslexics with MIS are subjects who have been diagnosed as dyslexic and use coloured filters to help in reading.

 

The breakdown of post hoc analysis showing the mean difference in TVSS, CQ and NCQ visual stress scores and the limits of confidence intervals (CI) are shown in Table 2. From the table, Group 2 subjects showed significantly higher TVSS (P=0.002), NCQ visual stress scores (P=0.006) and CQ visual stress scores (P=0.008) than control. In contrast, the TVSS, NCQ and CQ visual stress scores were similar between control and Group 1 subjects (P>0.05, for all comparisons). However, when this analysis was performed with Group 1 being split into dyslexic only and dyslexics with MIS, the TVSS (mean difference: -22; 95% CI: -42 to -2; P=0.024) and NCQ visual stress scores (mean difference: -11, 95% CI: -18 to -4; P=0.001) became significantly higher for dyslexics with MIS than control.

Table 2 Mean differences in mean scores between groups and 95% confidence intervals for critical question (CQ), non-critical question (NCQ) and total visual stress symptoms (TVSS)

Dependent variable

Group 1

Group 2

Mean difference

SE

P

95% confidence

interval

CQ

Control

Dyslexic with/without MIS

-0.217

3.723

0.998

-8.973, 8.538

MIS

-6.551

2.195

0.008

-11.712, -1.389

Dyslexic with/without MIS

Control

0.217

3.723

0.998

-8.538, 8.973

MIS

-6.333

4.253

0.297

-16.335, 3.669

MIS

Control

6.551

2.195

0.008

1.389, 11.712

Dyslexic with/without MIS

6.333

4.253

0.297

-3.668, 16.335

NCQ

Control

Dyslexic with/without MIS

-4.210

2.077

0.107

-9.094, 0.674

MIS

-3.766

1.224

0.006

-6.645, -0.887

Dyslexic with/without MIS

Control

4.210

2.077

0.107

-0.674, 9.094

MIS

0.444

2.373

0.981

-5.135, 6.023

MIS

Control

3.766

1.224

0.006

0.887, 6.645

Dyslexic with/without MIS

-0.444

2.372

0.981

-6.023, 5.135

TVSS

Control

Dyslexic with/without MIS

-4.428

5.156

0.667

-16.554, 7.699

MIS

-10.316

3.040

0.002

-17.465, -3.168

Dyslexic with/without MIS

Control

4.428

5.157

0.667

-7.699, 16.554

MIS

-5.889

5.891

0.577

-19.741, 7.963

MIS

Control

10.316

3.040

0.002

3.168, 17.465

Dyslexic with/without MIS

5.889

5.891

0.577

-7.963, 19.741

P values are results of post hoc analysis using Tukey’s HSD. The mean difference is significant at the 0.05 level.

 

DISCUSSION

Learning and reading disorders are common presentations in the primary care setting[29] and can have serious consequences on educational achievement of subjects. The need for increased awareness of symptoms and identification of patients has been identified[30]. In this study we assessed for the first time the frequency of occurrence of dyslexic and MIS among a Saudi literate population. The results show that about 8% of female medical students have reading difficulties. One in every 50 student (2%) is dyslexic and one in every 16 student (6%) has MIS. Although it has been difficult to assess the true prevalence of dyslexia mainly due to the differences in the definition of the condition, prevalence studies estimate that it affects between 4% and 10% of children[31-33] and higher rate of dyslexia has been reported among creative students than non-creative students[34]. In another study the prevalence of MIS in adult population was reported to be as common as in children[35]. Irlen[15] inferred that MIS affects 12% of the general population and the condition coexists in 65% of dyslexic people. In this study, we observed that a smaller proportion of dyslexic subjects (33%) also had MIS. Irlen’s observation were not backed up by actual data and as such cannot be compared with the current results. In addition to the subjective confirmation of MIS by the persistent benefit of coloured filters used during reading, or from immediate benefit that is indicated by increased reading rate[19,35], the respondents in this study also self-confirmed the diagnosis of the conditions, at the end of data collection. We included a large number of items to ensure content validity[36] as the study was designed to measure a particular trait in a given group of subjects (dyslexics).

Considering the very high completion rate recorded, it appears that the use of this questionnaire as a measure of visual stress level in dyslexic and MIS subjects was well received by the respondents in this study. It is possible that the high response rate was a reflection of the relevance of the subject being assessed to the respondents’ occupation (medical students faced with greater writing and reading task). Whether this survey will perform equally well in adults particularly with lesser education or of different occupation is subject to future research. Of the questions asked, enquiring on the respondents’ history of pattern glare (a symptom well known to be attributed to MIS condition) may not have been understood because only one positive response was received even though 27 respondents had MIS. Perhaps, the respondents were not familiar with the term although opportunity was made available for them during data collection to ask questions in order to clear any ambiguous issue, or those with MIS were unaware of this symptom. Pattern glare refers to the description of visual perceptual distortions, e.g. motion of the reading material, change in the spacing between letters and coloured halos, which in turn can cause visual discomfort, and asthenopic symptoms, by viewing certain striped pattern stimuli with spatial characteristics such as lines of the text on a page[19]. A revision of this term into a more familiar term in future studies may improve the respondents’ understanding of the symptom.

Visual stress which is the subjective experience of unpleasant visual symptoms when reading (especially for prolonged duration) and in response to some other visual stimuli[37] affects both dyslexics and non-dyslexic individuals[21]. It can be evaluated using objective and subjective techniques. Subjective assessment of visual stress level requires the use of appropriateness questions and in this study, significantly higher visual stress scores were observed in MIS subjects than control regardless of the questions implored as measurement tools. But, between the control and dyslexic subjects, we found no significant differences in mean scores when NCQ, CQ and TVSS were implored for assessment of visual stress. Additionally, we found that subjects with MIS were more sensitive to visual stress than normal subjects. More dyslexic adults (22%) than MIS (19%) and normal (11%) adults experienced transient blurring of near vision and about 56% of dyslexics, 18% of MIS and 24% of controls experienced double vision during prolonged reading (none of these symptoms differed between groups). von Károlyi and Winner[23] did not find any difference in the performance of perceptual and spatial tasks between dyslexic and non-dyslexic young adults. It has been reported that about 5% of all children and half of all dyslexic children complain of visual problems when they try to read and in these children, letters appear blurred or move around and go double while reading, making the children unable to see them properly[38]. Obviously, these symptoms will interfere with reading in these subjects[4] and may cause eyestrain and headaches. However, these may be reduced, in some cases, by wearing tinted lenses[39].

Various questionnaires have been used to assess visual stress in dyslexics[34,40-42]. Some have assessed the effects of coloured overlays on symptoms and reading rate[35] or measured the distribution of visual discomfort symptoms in dyslexics during reading[43]. While none of these studies have compared the level of visual stress in both groups (dyslexics and MIS subjects) in relation to controls, the responses from studies on children may have under-reported the symptoms due to a lack of understanding[44]. Children who constantly experience symptoms of visual stress during reading may not identify the symptom as unusual and therefore might accept it as normal, except perhaps if these symptoms are relieved by use of coloured overlays[35,45].

Scientists have searched for connections between hand preference and the presence of disorders that affect reading and language development. However, until now, no convincing evidence has been found. Recently, a genetic variant was discovered that appears to link handedness and reading ability. Children with a particular version of the gene, called PCSK6, have a right hand that is unusually dominant and are also poor at reading[46]. Most of the participants (90.9%) in this study were right handed (90.6% normal, 77.8% dyslexics and all MIS subjects) but this preference for hand and the other categorical variables like age and history of eye exercises were not significantly associated with either dyslexia or MIS. However, dyslexic subjects with or without MIS were significantly more likely to be diagnosed with lazy eyes than other subjects in this study. This is in agreement with a previous report that reduced amplitudes of convergence and accommodation were significantly correlated with dyslexia[47]. The authors also suggested that the binocular instability they observed in dyslexic subjects was due to the decreased amplitude of accommodation.

As a general drawback to all questionnaires, we could not expand the questions beyond the ones already asked and although we did not clinically examine these subjects in order to confirm actual diagnosis, the participants were contacted at the end of data collection and their self-reported diagnosis were confirmed. The decision to recruit only females in this study was pre-intended due to the high preponderance of visual stress symptom scores in women[48-49]. Although this may limit the generalization of the current results, the study highlights the importance of developing a standard set of questions for use in assessing visual stress among dyslexic and MIS adults as this will aid proper intervention in this group of subjects. 

In conclusion, we observed that dyslexia (with or without MIS) and MIS alone were present in 2% and 6% of adult female students in Saudi Arabia, respectively. Subjects with MIS were significantly more sensitive to visual stress than normal subjects but between dyslexic and normal subjects, the sensitivity to visual stress was similar. On the other hand, the dyslexic subjects were more likely to present with lazy eyes than other subjects and the use of critical questions was more appropriate for assessing visual stress levels in adult university students with reading difficulties.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Foundation: Supported by the Research Centre, College of Applied Medical Sciences and the Deanship of Scientific Research at King Saud University.

Conflicts of Interest: Alanazi MA, None; Alanazi SA, None; Osuagwu UL, None.


REFERENCES   [Top]

1 Voeller KK. Dyslexia. <ii>J Child Neurol </ii> 2004;19(10):740-744. [PubMed]

2 Tunmer W, Greaney K. Defining dyslexia. <ii>J Learn Disabil </ii> 2010;43(3):229-243. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

3 Bruce JWE. <ii>Dyslexia and Vision</ii>, Second edition. London and Philadelphia: Whurr, 2001.

4 Stein J. Dyslexia: the role of vision and visual attention. <ii>Curr Dev Disord Rep </ii> 2014;1(4):267-280. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [PMC free article]

5 Hoyles A, Hoyles M. Race and dyslexia. <ii>Race Ethnicity and Education</ii> 2010;13(2):209-231. [CrossRef]

6 Snowling MJ. <ii>Dyslexia</ii>: Blackwell, 2000;14:253.

7 Cornelissen P, Bradley L, Fowler S, Stein J. What children see affects how they spell. <ii>Dev Med Child Neurol </ii> 1994;36(8):716-726. [CrossRef]

8 Eperjesi F. Optometric assessment and management in dyslexia. <ii>Optometry Today</ii> 2000;40:20-25.

9 Evans B. The role of optometrist in dyslexia. Part 2: Optometric correlates of dyslexia. <ii>Optmetry Today</ii> 2004;44:35-39.

10 Hudson RF, High L, Al Otaiba S. Dyslexia and the brain: what does current research tell us? <ii>The Reading Teacher</ii> 2007;60(6):506-515. [CrossRef]

11 Schumacher J, Hoffmann P, Schmäl C, SchulteKörne G, Nöthen MM. Genetics of dyslexia: the evolving landscape. <ii>J Med Genet </ii> 2007;44(5):289-297. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [PMC free article]

12 Vellutino FR, Fletcher JM, Snowling MJ, Scanlon DM. Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? <ii>J Child Psychol Psychiatry</ii> 2004;45(1):2-40. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

13 Bishop DV, Snowling MJ. Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: same or different? <ii>Psychol Bull </ii> 2004;130(6):858-886. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

14 Plomin R, Kovas Y. Generalist genes and learning disabilities. <ii>Psychol Bull </ii> 2005;131(4):592-617. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

15 Irlen H. <ii>Reading by the Colours: Overcoming Dyslexia and other reading disabilities Through the Irlen Method. </ii>New York: A very Publishing Group;1991.

16 Chang M, Kim SH, Kim JY, Cho YA. Specific visual symptoms and signs of Meares-Irlen syndrome in Korean. <ii>Korean J Ophthalmol </ii> 2014;28(2):159-163. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [PMC free article]

17 Uccula A, Enna M, Mulatti C. Colors, colored overlays, and reading skills. <ii>Front Psychol </ii> 2014;5:833. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [PMC free article]

18 Kriss I, Evans BJW. The relationship between dyslexia and MearesIrlen Syndrome. <ii>Journal of Research in Reading</ii> 2005;28(3):350-364. [CrossRef]

19 Conlon EG, Sanders MA, Wright CM. Relationships between global motion and global form processing, practice, cognitive and visual processing in adults with dyslexia or visual discomfort. <ii>Neuropsychologia</ii> 2009;47(3):907-915. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

20 Wilkins AJ, Evans BJ. Visual stress, its treatment with spectral filters, and its relationship to visually induced motion sickness. <ii>Appl Ergon </ii> 2010;41(4):509-515. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

21 Singleton C, Henderson LM. Computerized screening for visual stress in children with dyslexia. <ii>Dyslexia</ii> 2007;13(2):130-151. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

22 Kruk R, Sumbler K, Willows D. Visual processing characteristics of children with Meares-Irlen syndrome. <ii>Ophthalmic Physiol Opt </ii> 2008;28(1):35-46. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

23 von Károlyi C, Winner E. Dyslexia and visual spatial talents: are they connected? <ii>Neuropsychology and Cognition</ii> 2004;25:95-117. [CrossRef]

24 Singleton C, Trotter S. Visual stress in adults with and without dyslexia. <ii>Journal of Research in Reading</ii> 2005;28(3):365-378. [CrossRef]

25 Nichols S. Supporting dyslexic adults in higher education and the workplace. <ii>John Wiley & Sons</ii> 2012:33-42. [CrossRef]

26 Evans BJ. The need for optometric investigation in suspected Meares-Irlen syndrome or visual stress. <ii>Ophthalmic Physiol Opt </ii> 2005;25(4):363-370. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

27 Morris DK, Turnbull PA. The disclosure of dyslexia in clinical practice: experiences of student nurses in the United Kingdom. <ii>Nurse Educ Today</ii> 2007;27(1):35-42. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

28 Mumtaz Y, Jahangeer SMA, Mujtaba T, Zafar S, Adnan S. Self medication among university students of Karachi. <ii>J Liaquat Uni Med Health Sci</ii> 2011;10(3):102-105.

29 Wright C. Learning disorders, dyslexia, and vision. <ii>Aust Fam Physician </ii> 2007;36(10):843-845. [PubMed]

30 Crabtree E. Educational implications of Meares-Irlen syndrome. <ii>British Journal of School Nursing</ii> 2011;6(4):182-187. [CrossRef]

31 Sun Z, Zou L, Zhang J, Mo S, Shao S, Zhong R, Ke J, Lu X, Miao X, Song R. Prevalence and associated risk factors of dyslexic children in a middle-sized city of china: a cross-sectional study. <ii>PLoS One</ii> 2013;8(2):e56688. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [PMC free article]

32 Barbiero C, Lonciari I, Montico M, <ii>et al</ii>. The submerged dyslexia iceberg: how many school children are not diagnosed? Results from an Italian Study.<ii> PLoS One</ii> 2012;7(10):e48082.

33 Paulesu E, Démonet JF, Fazio F, McCrory E, Chanoine V, Brunswick N, Cappa SF, Cossu G, Habib M, Frith CD, Frith U. Dyslexia: cultural diversity and biological unity. <ii>Science</ii> 2001;291(5511):2165-2167. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

34 Wolff U, Lundberg I. The prevalence of dyslexia among art students. <ii>Dyslexia</ii> 2002;8(1):34-42. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

35 Evans BJ, Joseph F. The effect of coloured filters on the rate of reading in an adult student population. <ii>Ophthalmic Physiol Opt </ii> 2002;22(6):535-545. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

36 Streiner DL, Norman GR, Cairney J. <ii>Health measurment scales:a prectical guide to their development and use. Fifth edition</ii>. New York: Oxford University Press, 2014. [CrossRef]

37 Singleton C. Visual stress and dyslexia. <ii>The Routledge Companion to Dyslexia. </ii>London:<ii> </ii>Routledge, 2009:43-57. [CrossRef]

38 Wilkins A, Huang J, Cao Y. Visual stress theory and its application to reading and reading tests. <ii>Journal of Research in Reading</ii> 2004;27(2):152-162. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

39 Wilkins AJ, Patel R, Adjamian P, Evans BJ. Tinted spectacles and visually sinsitive migraine. <ii>Cephalalgia</ii> 2002;22(9):711-719. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

40 Logan J. Dyslexic entrepreneurs: the incidence; their coping strategies and their business skills. <ii>Dyslexia</ii> 2009;15(4):328-346. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

41 Undheim AM. A thirteenyear followup study of young Norwegian adults with dyslexia in childhood: reading development and educational levels. <ii>Dyslexia</ii> 2009;15(4):291-303. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

42 Kujala T, Halmetoja J, Näätänen R, Alku P, Lyytinen H Sussman E. Speech and soundsegmentation in dyslexia: evidence for a multiplelevel cortical impairment. <ii>Eur J Neurosci</ii> 2006;24(8):2420-2427. [CrossRef]

43 Borsting E, Chase C, Tosha C, Ridder WH 3rd. Longitudinal study of visual discomfort symptoms in college students. <ii>Optom Vis Sci </ii> 2008;85(10):992-998. [CrossRef]

44 Vidyasagar TR. Neural underpinnings of dyslexia as a disorder of visuospatial attention. <ii>Clin Exp Optom </ii> 2004;87(1):4-10.

47 Handler SM, Fierson WM, Section on Ophthalmology; Council on Children with Disabilities; American Academy of Ophthalmology; American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus; American Association of Certified Orthoptists. Learning disorders, dyslexia, and vision. Pediatrics  2011;127(3):e818-856.

48 Borsting E, Chase CH, Ridder WH 3rd. Measuring visual discomfort in college students. Optom Vis Sci  2007;84(8):745-751.

49 Alanazi Mana A. Studies of visual functions and the effect of visual fatigue in adults with dyslexia. United Kingdom: Cardiff University, 2010:1-309.

  [Top]