bannter.jpg
·
Review Article·

 

Potential of video games for the promotion of neuroadaptation to multifocal intraocular lenses: a narrative review

 

María Begoña Coco-Martin1,2, Pedro L. Valenzuela3, Miguel J. Maldonado-López2, Alejandro Santos-Lozano4, Ainhoa Molina-Martín5, David P Piñero5,6

 

1Research Unit, Hospital Clínico Universitario de Valladolid. ICSCYL, Valladolid 47003, Spain

2Grupo de Cirugía Refractiva y Rehabilitación Visual. Instituto Universitario de Oftalmobiología Aplicada (IOBA), Universidad de Valladolid, Valladolid 47011, Spain

3Physiology Unit, Systems Biology Department, University of Alcalá, Madrid 28801, Spain

4Grupo de Investigación i+Health, Departamento de Ciencias de la Salud, Universidad Europea Miguel de Cervantes, Valladolid 47012, Spain

5Department of Optics, Pharmacology and Anatomy, University of Alicante, Alicante 03690, Spain

6Department of Ophthalmology (IMQO-Oftalmar), Vithas Medimar International Hospital, Alicante 03016, Spain

Correspondence to: David P. Piñero. Department of Optics, Pharmacology and Anatomy, University of Alicante, Crta San Vicente del Raspeig s/n, 03690 San Vicente del Raspeig, Alicante 03690, Spain. david.pinyero@ua.es

Received: 2019-02-15        Accepted: 2019-06-11

 

Abstract

Multifocal intraocular lenses (IOLs) are currently usually implanted for the treatment of cataracts because they have been proved to be superior to monofocal IOLs with respect to spectacle independence. In turn, they are associated with a higher prevalence of dysphotopsia symptoms that is one of the most common causes of patient dissatisfaction. Neuroadaptation seems to play a major role in the optimal adaptation to multifocal IOLs. In this context, the development of strategies that facilitate the neuroadaptation process to multifocality might be an effective strategy to reduce patients’ dissatisfaction. Video games have been proved to be effective for the improvement of visual acuity and for the promotion of neuroplasticity in elderly subjects and other populations with cortical-related visual impairment. This narrative review highlights the physiological potential of video games as a perceptual strategy to improve visual acuity and promote neuroplasticity in patients using multifocal IOLs, although research is still needed to confirm these benefits in this specific population, with only one comparative study to this date providing evidence of them.

KEYWORDS: multifocal intraocular lens; dysphotopsia; photic phenomena; halos; video games; neuroadaptation; neuroplasticity

DOI:10.18240/ijo.2019.11.18

 

Citation: Coco-Martín MB, Valenzuela PL, Maldonado-López MJ, Santos-Lozano A, Molina-Martín A, Piñero DP. Potential of video games for the promotion of neuroadaptation to multifocal intraocular lenses: a narrative review. Int J Ophthalmol 2019;12(11):1782-1787

 

INTRODUCTION

Societies are progressively aging, which comes accompanied by an increased prevalence of age-related diseases[1]. Cataracts, that is, the degeneration and subsequent opacity of crystalline lens, are one of these diseases. In fact, they are among the leading causes of visual impairment, especially in developing countries[2-3].

Cataract surgery techniques have progressively improved, with around 90% of patients presenting optimal vision acuity afterwards[4]. The standard intraocular lenses (IOLs) traditionally implanted (monofocal IOLs) during cataract surgery have fixed refractive power and focal length, and consequently most patients using these IOLs need glasses to improve their vision at certain distances. In turn, multifocal IOLs offer full refractive correction at all distances, and are therefore superior to monofocal IOLs with respect to spectacle independence[5]. However, multifocal IOLs are associated with a higher prevalence of visual disturbances (e.g., glare and haloes)[5], being low visual acuity and specially dysphotopsia symptoms the main causes of dissatisfaction in patients using these lenses[6-7].

The causes of the abovementioned multifocal-related visual problems remain unclear. Parameters such as visual acuity and other causes of impaired vision such as dry eye or retinal disease seem not to be related to the incidence of dysphotopsic symptoms and the associated dissatisfaction[6,8]. In turn, it has been recently suggested that the activation of certain brain areas might play a role in the adaptation to multifocal IOLs. There is an association between multifocal IOL users’ difficulties and the activation of cortical areas dedicated to attention (i.e. frontoparietal circuits), learning and cognitive control (i.e. cingulate), and task goals (i.e. caudate)[8]. Indeed, improving visual attention and procedural learning networks seems to be an essential part of the initial stages of neuroadaptation to multifocal IOLs, as these neural changes are associated with improvements in symptoms, visual acuity and contrast sensitivity independently of optical factors[9].

In this context, the development of strategies that facilitate the neuroadaptation process to multifocal IOLs might be an effective approach to reduce patients’ dissatisfaction. The aim of this narrative review was to summarize the evidence on the potential effectiveness of video games for the promotion of neuroadaptation in the general population as well as in individuals with vision impairment, as well as to describe the characteristics of those video games that have shown beneficial effects. For this purpose, we performed a non-systematic review in PubMed using the terms “video games” or “computerized cognitive training” and terms such as brain plasticity, neuroplasticity, neuroadaptation, cognition, visual acuity, vision, visual impairment, cataract or amblyopia. Reference lists of relevant articles and reviews were also examined to find additional publications on the topic.


VIDEO GAMES, A POSSIBLE SOLUTION

The Role of Videogames in Neuroplasticity and Vision  In this context, the development of strategies that facilitate the neuroadaptation process to multifocal IOLs might be an effective approach to reduce patients’ dissatisfaction. Video games have been proved to promote brain plasticity. For instance, increases in gray matter in areas crucial for spatial navigation, strategic planning, working memory and motor performance (i.e., right hippocampal formation, right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and bilateral cerebellum) have been observed in young subjects after two months of video game playing[10]. Similarly, other authors reported an enhanced functional connectivity and grey matter volume in insular sub-regions and between the attentional and sensorimotor networks in young video game players[11]. Gaming can promote brain adaptations and improve functional connectivity even at advanced age[12]. It has proven to attenuate age-related deficits in neural signatures of cognitive control-reflected by enhanced midline frontal theta power and frontal posterior theta coherence measured by means of electroencephalography-as well as to improve sustained attention and working memory in older adults[13]. Moreover, increases in neural activity (anterior P3a amplitude) have been observed after a five-week intervention with a similar strategy, computerized cognitive training[14].

Owing to the effects that these ‘therapies’ elicit in a variety of brain functions such as attention, cognitive control and visuospatial skills[15], gaming and computerized strategies provide several benefits in cognition and vision in elderly subjects[16-20]. Moreover, not only they can improve vision in healthy individuals but also in clinical conditions, such as amblyopia[21]. Amblyopia is a developmental abnormality characterized by an alteration in the visual cortex that results in vision impairment (i.e., low visual acuity and contrast sensitivity, and perception of crowding effect and suppression). The traditional treatment for childhood amblyopia was occlusion therapy (i.e., patching the non-amblyopic eye), but more recent evidence demonstrated that the inclusion of perceptual strategies such as playing video games with the amblyopic eye can result in a more rapid improvement[22]. Indeed, it was traditionally believed that amblyopia was irreversible beyond the sensitive period of brain development during childhood. However, recent studies demonstrate that perceptual training strategies can also enhance visual plasticity and improve visual acuity and contrast sensitivity in adults with amblyopia[23-24].

Characteristics of Potentially Effective Video Games for the Promotion of Neuroadaptation  Different types of video games can improve vision and cognitive function. Action video games, ones of the most popular among young subjects, are probably the most effective for this purpose, having proven to promote brain plasticity and enhance different variables such as speed of processing, perception, attention or cognition[25-27]. However, most action video games share some characteristics (are too fast, intense, unpredictable and sometimes even violent) that make them unsuitable for older adults, the population that most commonly suffer from cataracts. Indeed, this type of video games has been reported as less interesting -inducing therefore a lower compliance- than non-action video games in elderly adults[28-31], being intellectually stimulating games (e.g., puzzle, simulation and strategy ones) the most appealing[28,32].

In this sense, non-action gaming strategies that include cognitive training for the improvement of mental abilities (the so called ‘brain training’ games), such as Nintendo game Brain Age or the commercial games Lumosity (https://www.lumosity.com), Cogmed (https://www.cogmed.com) or Fit Brains (http://www.fitbrains.com), which are also available as smartphone applications, have shown promising results in elderly subjects. This type of games has been reported to improve executive function, processing speed, attention and visual recognition memory, and working memory in this population[14,33-37], being the benefits greater than those provided by other common games such as the falling blocks (Tetris)[37]. Other authors observed a reduction of distraction and an increase of alertness in older adults after 20 one-hour game training sessions compared to a control group[38]. Moreover, improvements in inhibition and inductive reasoning have also been reported in these individuals after this type of training when compared to a control group that watched a documentary[39]. Some benefits can also be obtained with driving simulation games, which have been reported to improve multi-task performance, cognitive control, enhanced sustained attention and working memory in older adults[13].

In summary, non-action video games such as simulation and especially cognitive training ones appear as a promising strategy for the improvement of vision in elderly subjects. However, as abovementioned, the impaired vision of individuals with multifocal IOLs seem to be largely related to a new and non-physiological focusing properties of the retinal image formation that need development of novel suppression and filtering strategies in the brain cortex, and consequently analyzing the gaming strategies and cognitive stimuli that have previously demonstrated to be effective in visual impairments related to cortical alteration, such as amblyopia, could be especially useful for the optimal design of video games in this population.

Perceptual training, which can be conducted through playing video games, improves visual acuity in amblyopia, reducing spatial distortion (internal neural noise) and increasing sampling efficiency (the ability to extract stimulus information)[40]. Playing video games using only the amblyopic eye (i.e., monocular training) has proven to enhance visual and positional acuity, spatial attention and stereopsis in adults through these neural adaptations[24]. However, a binocular alternative approach known as dichoptic training has also been recently proposed. During this intervention, high and low contrast stimuli are presented to the amblyopic and fellow eye, respectively, in order to balance the input from both and enable binocular integration. This strategy has also been reported to improve contrast sensitivity and visual acuity in adults with amblyopia[41-42], and seems to be more effective for the improvement of several visual functions than other strategies such as watching movies with a patch on the non-amblyopic eye[43] or monocular training[44].

Thus, dichoptic training providing different contrast stimuli for the operated and the fellow eye might be a potentially effective strategy for the promotion of neuroadaptation to multifocal IOLs, and consequently for the enhancement of visual acuity. For example, games in which subjects drive a vehicle and some objects are just seen with the ‘bad’ eye have been proved to be beneficial in amblyopia[45]. Similarly, the Tetris game with some blocks being presented to both eyes but others just to the amblyopic or the fellow eye at high and low contrast, respectively, is also an alternative strategy for the improvement of vision[41,46].

Therapies including perceptual learning signals have also shown remarkable results in subjects with myopia and amblyopia[23,44,47], and could provide benefits in patients with multifocal IOLs. In these games, subjects usually have to take decisions (e.g., decide which button to click) depending on the Gabor patch presented, with Gabor stimuli varying in number, spatial arrangement, global and local orientation, target-flankers separation, exposure time or contrast and spatial frequency. Moreover, the combination of dichoptic training with perceptual learning might maximize the benefits[43-44]. Following the study of Vedamurthy et al[43] who demonstrated that this strategy is effective for the improvement of visual acuity in amblyopic adults, a discrimination perceptual learning task (e.g., Gabbor patch) can be presented to the operated eye during the game so that the subject rapidly performs an action (e.g., clicking a button or not) depending on its orientation.

Previous Experiences of Visual Training in Eyes Implanted with Multifocal IOLs  The scientific evidence to this date of the use of visual training to improve the results of eyes undergoing cataract surgery with implantation of multifocal IOLs is scarce[48-49]. Kaymak et al[48] evaluated the effect of computer-based visual training in only one eye (with the fellow eye occluded) of patients undergoing bilateral cataract surgery with implantation of multifocal IOLs (apodized diffractive and bifocal diffractive designs). The untrained fellow eye served as control. The training was performed during a 6-week postoperative period and based on the concept of perceptual learning of discrimination line orientations. The presentation of the stimuli was done at a fixed distance of 1 m from the observer in a LCD 19” monitor (1280×1024 resolution, 75 Hz). These were the main characteristics of the stimuli presented: thin bright lines 1 arcmin wide presented on a dark surround, length of the lines of 50 arcmin, orientation of the lines controlled by a staircase procedure, and presentation time of 500ms. The patient had to decide in each presentation in which direction the bars were tilted by pushing either the left or right of two push buttons (left/right for vertical stimuli and left/right end lower to the other end for horizontal stimuli). The pause between stimulus presentation was of 300ms. The training was performed over 2wk in six sessions in one eye of each patient, with 7 blocks with 50 stimulus presentations each to be fulfilled in each session. Only one session per observer took place each day, with sessions following each other in intervals of no more than 3 subsequent days. The authors reported a significantly faster and larger improvement of orientation visual acuity, contrast sensitivity and near vision in the trained eyes compared to control eyes, with a mean duration of training sessions of 30±5min[48]. The superior function of the trained eyes was still present at 6mo after surgery[48]. A summary of this same research was reported by this research group in another journal, indicating that the mean improvement of orientation discrimination in the trained eyes was 44%, which was significantly higher compared with the control eyes (9%)[49]. The potential effect of this training may have been magnified by gamifying the training procedure. This combination of visual training based on perceptual learning and gamification is a potential research area that may facilitate a better and more satisfactory visual outcome of eyes undergoing cataract surgery with implantation of multifocal IOLs.


CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE PERSPECTIVES

The popularity of multifocal IOLs implantation after cataract surgery is rapidly growing because they have proven superior to monofocal IOLs with respect to spectacle independence. However, they are associated with a higher prevalence of other complications such as low visual acuity and visual complains (e.g., dysphotopsia symptoms).

Promoting neuroadaptation to multifocal IOLs seems to play a major role in the presence of symptoms in patients using these lenses, and video games appear as a promising strategy for this purpose (Figure 1). As shown in this narrative review, some evidence suggests that gaming strategies might be effective for the promotion of neuroplasticity and for the improvement of vision in elderly subjects and in populations with cortical-related visual impairments such as amblyopia. Games including cognitive training stimuli seem to be the most effective for the enhancement of neuroplasticity in elderly subjects, as well as the most appealing to this population. These benefits might be applicable to patients using multifocal IOLs, as demonstrated initially by a first comparative study. The benefit in these eyes may be in terms of visual performance as well as in terms of less perception of photic phenomena.

Maria Begona Coco-Martin1

Figure 1 Graphic summary of multifocal IOLs-related pros and cons, and of the potential benefits of video games in these patients.

 

Notwithstanding, despite the potential of video games for the promotion of neuroadaptation to multifocal IOLs, research is still needed to confirm if they can effectively improve visual acuity in these patients and decrease multifocal IOLs-related adverse effects or reduce the postoperative time to reach satisfaction with vision performance. Specifically, clinical trials are needed to provide more consistent evidence of the use of this type of training with video games in eyes implanted with multifocal IOLs.


ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This article is part of the research performed in the context of the Project OPTiTRAIN, (“Neuroentrenamiento visual en pacientes operados de cirugía de cataratas e implantados con lente intraocular multifocal”, application number 106153).

Foundation: Coco-Martin MB, Maldonado-López MJ, Molina-Martín A and Piñero DP are supported by CDTI (Centro para el Desarrollo Tecnológico Industrial, Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness of Spain) by means of the program PID (“Proyectos de Investigación y Desarrollo”); The author Piñero DP has been also supported by the Ministry of Economy, Industry and Competitiveness of Spain within the program Ramón y Cajal (RYC-2016-20471); Valenzuela PL is supported by a contract granted by University of Alcalá (FPI2016).

Conflicts of Interest: Coco-Martín MB, None; Valenzuela PL, None; Maldonado-López MJ, None; Santos-Lozano A, None; Molina-Martín A, None; Piñero DP, None.


References

1 World Health Organization. Global Health and Aging. WHO; US National Institute of Aging 2011, doi:11-7737. https://www.who.int/ageing/publications/global_health.pdf. Accessed on Oct. 26, 2018.

 

2 Prokofyeva E, Wegener A, Zrenner E. Cataract prevalence and prevention in Europe: a literature review. Acta Ophthalmol 2013;91(5): 395-405.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1755-3768.2012.02444.x
PMid:22715900

 

3 Abraham AG, Condon NG, West Gower E. The new epidemiology of cataract. Ophthalmol Clin North Am 2006;19(4):415-425.

 

4 Allen D, Vasavada A. Cataract and surgery for cataract. BMJ 2006;333(7559):128-132.
https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.333.7559.128
PMid:16840470 PMCid:PMC1502210

 

5 de Silva SR, Evans JR, Kirthi V, Ziaei M, Leyland M. Multifocal versus monofocal intraocular lenses after cataract extraction. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2016;12:CD003169.
https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD003169.pub4
PMCid:PMC6463930

 

6 Kinard K, Jarstad A, Olson RJ. Correlation of visual quality with satisfaction and function in a normal cohort of pseudophakic patients. J Cataract Refract Surg 2013;39(4):590-597.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrs.2012.11.023
PMid:23395326

 

7 Woodward MA, Randleman JB, Stulting RD. Dissatisfaction after multifocal intraocular lens implantation. J Cataract Refract Surg 2009;35(6):992-997.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrs.2009.01.031
PMid:19465282 PMCid:PMC5125020

 

8 Rosa AM, Miranda ÂC, Patrício M, McAlinden C, Silva FL, Murta JN, Castelo-Branco M. Functional magnetic resonance imaging to assess the neurobehavioral impact of dysphotopsia with multifocal intraocular lenses. Ophthalmology 2017;124(9):1280-1289.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ophtha.2017.03.033
PMid:28433446

 

9 Rosa AM, Miranda ÂC, Patrício MM, McAlinden C, Silva FL, Castelo-Branco M, Murta JN. Functional magnetic resonance imaging to assess neuroadaptation to multifocal intraocular lenses. J Cataract Refract Surg 2017;43(10):1287-1296.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrs.2017.07.031
PMid:29120714

 

10 Kühn S, Gleich T, Lorenz RC, Lindenberger U, Gallinat J. Playing Super Mario induces structural brain plasticity: gray matter changes resulting from training with a commercial video game. Mol Psychiatry 2014;19(2):265-271.
https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2013.120
PMid:24166407

 

11 Gong D, He H, Liu D, Ma W, Dong L, Luo C, Yao D. Enhanced functional connectivity and increased gray matter volume of insula related to action video game playing. Sci Rep 2015;5:9763.
https://doi.org/10.1038/srep09763
PMid:25880157 PMCid:PMC5381748

 

12 Ten Brinke LF, Davis JC, Barha CK, Liu-Ambrose T. Effects of computerized cognitive training on neuroimaging outcomes in older adults: a systematic review. BMC Geriatr 2017;17(1):139.
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-017-0529-x
PMid:28693437 PMCid:PMC5504817

 

13 Anguera JA, Boccanfuso J, Rintoul JL, Al-Hashimi O, Faraji F, Janowich J, Kong E, Larraburo Y, Rolle C, Johnston E, Gazzaley A. Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature 2013;501(7465):97-101.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12486
PMid:24005416 PMCid:PMC3983066

 

14 Tusch ES, Alperin BR, Ryan E, Holcomb PJ, Mohammed AH, Daffner KR. Changes in neural activity underlying working memory after computerized cognitive training in older adults. Front Aging Neurosci 2016;8:255.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2016.00255
PMid:27877122 PMCid:PMC5099139

 

15 Palaus M, Marron EM, Viejo-Sobera R, Redolar-Ripoll D. Neural basis of video gaming: a systematic review. Front Hum Neurosci 2017;11:248.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00248
PMid:28588464 PMCid:PMC5438999

 

16 Shah TM, Weinborn M, Verdile G, Sohrabi HR, Martins RN. Enhancing cognitive functioning in healthly older adults: a systematic review of the clinical significance of commercially available computerized cognitive training in preventing cognitive decline. Neuropsychol Rev 2017;27(1):62-80.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-016-9338-9
PMid:28092015

 

17 Wang P, Liu HH, Zhu XT, Meng T, Li HJ, Zuo XN. Action video game training for healthy adults: a Meta-analytic study. Front Psychol 2016;7:907.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00907
PMid:27378996 PMCid:PMC4911405

 

18 Toril P, Reales JM, Ballesteros S. Video game training enhances cognition of older adults: a Meta-analytic study. Psychol Aging 2014;29(3):706-716.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037507
PMid:25244488

 

19 Latham AJ, Patston LL, Tippett LJ. The virtual brain: 30 years of video-game play and cognitive abilities. Front Psychol 2013;4:629.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00629
PMid:24062712 PMCid:PMC3772618

 

20 Kueider AM, Parisi JM, Gross AL, Rebok GW. Computerized cognitive training with older adults: a systematic review. PLoS One 2012;7(7):e40588.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040588
PMid:22792378 PMCid:PMC3394709

 

21 Foss AJ. Use of video games for the treatment of amblyopia. Curr Opin Ophthalmol 2017;28(3):276-281.
https://doi.org/10.1097/ICU.0000000000000358
PMid:28141763

 

22 Kelly KR, Jost RM, Dao L, Beauchamp CL, Leffler JN, Birch EE. Binocular ipad game vs patching for treatment of amblyopia in children: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Ophthalmol 2016;134(12):1402-1408.
https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2016.4224
PMid:27832248 PMCid:PMC6054712

 

23 Yalcin E, Balci O. Efficacy of perceptual vision therapy in enhancing visual acuity and contrast sensitivity function in adult hypermetropic anisometropic amblyopia. Clin Ophthalmol 2014;8:49-53.
https://doi.org/10.2147/OPTH.S48300
PMid:24376340 PMCid:PMC3864996

 

24 Li RW, Ngo C, Nguyen J, Levi DM. Video-game play induces plasticity in the visual system of adults with amblyopia. PLoS Biol 2011;9(8):e1001135.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001135
PMid:21912514 PMCid:PMC3166159

 

25 Green CS, Bavelier D. Learning, attentional control, and action video games. Curr Biol 2012;22(6):R197-R206.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.02.012
PMid:22440805 PMCid:PMC3461277

 

26 Dye MW, Green CS, Bavelier D. Increasing speed of processing with action video games. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2009;18(6):321-326.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01660.x
PMid:20485453 PMCid:PMC2871325

 

27 Achtman RL, Green CS, Bavelier D. Video games as a tool to train visual skills. Restor Neurol Neurosci 2008;26(4-5):435-446.

 

28 Blocker KA, Wright TJ, Boot WR. Gaming preferences of aging generations. Gerontechnology 2014;12(3):174-184.
https://doi.org/10.4017/gt.2014.12.3.008.00
PMid:29033699

 

29 Boot WR, Champion M, Blakely DP, Wright T, Souders DJ, Charness N. Video games as a means to reduce age-related cognitive decline: attitudes, compliance, and effectiveness. Front Psychol 2013;4:31.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00031

 

30 McKay SM, Maki BE. Attitudes of older adults toward shooter video games: an initial study to select an acceptable game for training visual processing. Gerontechnology 2010;9(1):5-17.
https://doi.org/10.4017/gt.2010.09.01.001.00
PMid:22973185

 

31 Nap HH, Kort YAW, Ijsselsteijn WA. Senior gamers: preferences, motivations and needs. Gerontechnology 2009;8(4):247-262.
https://doi.org/10.4017/gt.2009.08.04.003.00

 

32 Chesham A, Wyss P, Müri RM, Mosimann UP, Nef T. What older people like to play: genre preferences and acceptance of casual games. JMIR Serious Games 2017;5(2):e8.
https://doi.org/10.2196/games.7025
PMid:28420601 PMCid:PMC5413800

 

33 McLaughlin PM, Curtis AF, Branscombe-Caird LM, Comrie JK, Murtha SJE. The feasibility and potential impact of brain training games on cognitive and emotional functioning in middle-aged adults. Games Health J 2018;7(1):67-74.
https://doi.org/10.1089/g4h.2017.0032
PMid:29189046 PMCid:PMC5797320

 

34 Oh SJ, Seo S, Lee JH, Song MJ, Shin MS. Effects of smartphone-based memory training for older adults with subjective memory complaints: a randomized controlled trial. Aging Ment Health 2017; 22(4):526-534.
https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2016.1274373
PMid:28071929

 

35 Toril P, Reales JM, Mayas J, Ballesteros S. Video game training enhances visuospatial working memory and episodic memory in older adults. Front Hum Neurosci 2016;10:206.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00206
PMid:27199723 PMCid:PMC4859063

 

36 Ballesteros S, Prieto A, Mayas J, Toril P, Pita C, Ponce de León L, Reales JM, Waterworth J. Brain training with non-action video games enhances aspects of cognition in older adults: a randomized controlled trial. Front Aging Neurosci 2014;6:277.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2014.00277
PMid:25352805 PMCid:PMC4196565

 

37 Nouchi R, Taki Y, Takeuchi H, Hashizume H, Akitsuki Y, Shigemune Y, Sekiguchi A, Kotozaki Y, Tsukiura T, Yomogida Y, Kawashima R. Brain training game improves executive functions and processing speed in the elderly: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS One 2012; 7(1):e29676.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0029676
PMid:22253758 PMCid:PMC3256163

 

38 Mayas J, Parmentier FB, Andrés P, Ballesteros S. Plasticity of attentional functions in older adults after non-action video game training: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS One 2014;9(3):e92269.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0092269
PMid:24647551 PMCid:PMC3960226

 

39 van Muijden J, Band GP, Hommel B. Online games training aging brains: limited transfer to cognitive control functions. Front Hum Neurosci 2012;6:221.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00221
PMid:22912609 PMCid:PMC3421963

 

40 Levi DM, Li RW. Perceptual learning as a potential treatment for amblyopia: a mini-review. Vision Res 2009;49(21):2535-2549.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.02.010
PMid:19250947 PMCid:PMC2764839

 

41 Li J, Spiegel DP, Hess RF, Chen Z, Chan LY, Deng D, Yu M, Thompson B. Dichoptic training improves contrast sensitivity in adults with amblyopia. Vision Res 2015;114:161-172.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2015.01.017
PMid:25676883

 

42 Vedamurthy I, Nahum M, Bavelier D, Levi DM. Mechanisms of recovery of visual function in adult amblyopia through a tailored action video game. Sci Rep 2015;5:8482.
https://doi.org/10.1038/srep08482
PMid:25719537 PMCid:PMC4894407

 

43 Vedamurthy I, Nahum M, Huang SJ, Zheng F, Bayliss J, Bavelier D, Levi DM. A dichoptic custom-made action video game as a treatment for adult amblyopia. Vision Res 2015;114:173-187.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2015.04.008
PMid:25917239 PMCid:PMC4549206

 

44 Liu XY, Zhang JY. Dichoptic training in adults with amblyopia: additional stereoacuity gains over monocular training. Vision Res 2018;152:84-90.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2017.07.002
PMid:28736224

 

45 Žiak P, Holm A, Halička J, Mojžiš P, Piñero DP. Amblyopia treatment of adults with dichoptic training using the virtual reality oculus rift head mounted display: preliminary results. BMC Ophthalmol 2017;17(1):105.
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12886-017-0501-8
PMid:28659140 PMCid:PMC5490155

 

46 Hess RF, Babu RJ, Clavagnier S, Black J, Bobier W, Thompson B. The iPod binocular home-based treatment for amblyopia in adults: efficacy and compliance. Clin Exp Optom 2014;97(5):389-398.
https://doi.org/10.1111/cxo.12192
PMid:25131694

 

47 Tan DT, Fong A. Efficacy of neural vision therapy to enhance contrast sensitivity function and visual acuity in low myopia. J Cataract Refract Surg 2008;34(4):570-577.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrs.2007.11.052
PMid:18361977

 

48 Kaymak H, Fahle M, Ott G, Mester U. Intraindividual comparison of the effect of training on visual performance with ReSTOR and Tecnis diffractive multifocal IOLs. J Refract Surg 2008;24(3):287-293.
https://doi.org/10.3928/1081597X-20080301-11
PMid:18416264

 

49 Mester U, Fahle M, Ott G, Kaymak H. Functional vision training after MIOL implantation. Ophthalmologe 2008;105(6):533-537.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00347-008-1746-7
PMid:18516604