bannter.jpg
Citation: Chen AH, Khalid NM, Buari NH. Age factor affects reading acuity and reading speed in attaining text information. Int J Ophthalmol 2019;12(7):1170-1176
DOI:10.18240/ijo.2019.07.19


¡¤
Investigation¡¤

Age factor affects reading acuity and reading speed in attaining text information

Ai-Hong Chen, Nursyairah Mohd Khalid, Noor Halilah Buari

Optometry, Faculty of Health Sciences, Universiti Teknologi MARA, Cawangan Selangor, Kampus Puncak Alam 42300, Malaysia

Correspondence to: Ai-Hong Chen. Optometry, Faculty of Health Sciences, Universiti Teknologi MARA, Cawangan Selangor, Kampus Puncak Alam 42300, Malaysia. aihong0707@yahoo.com

Received: 2018-03-11        Accepted: 2018-10-22

Abstract

AIM: To investigate the effect of age on reading acuity and reading speed in attaining text information in healthy eyes.

METHODS: Reading acuity, critical print size, reading speed and maximum reading speed were measured in groups of 40 children (8 to 12 years old), 40 teenagers (13 to 19 years old), 40 young adults (20 to 39 years old), and 40 adults (40 years old and above) using the Buari-Chen Malay Reading Chart [contextual sentences (CS) set and random words (RW) set] in a cross-sectional study design.

RESULTS: Reading acuity was significantly improved by 0.04 logMAR for both CS set and RW set from children to teenagers, then gradually worsened from young adults to adults (CS set: 0.06 logMAR; RW set: 0.08 logMAR). Critical print size for children showed a significant improvement in teenagers (CS set: 0.14 logMAR; RW set: 0.07 logMAR), then deteriorated from young adults to adults by 0.09 logMAR only for CS set. Reading speed significantly increased from children to teenagers, [CS set: 46.20 words per minute (wpm); RW set: 42.06 wpm], then stabilized from teenagers to young adults, and significantly reduced from young adults to adults (CS set: 28.58 wpm; RW set: 24.44 wpm). Increment and decrement in maximum reading speed measurement were revealed from children to teenagers (CS set: 39.38 wpm; RW set: 43.38 wpm) and from young adults to adults (CS set: 22.26 wpm; RW set: 26.31 wpm) respectively.

CONCLUSION: The reference of age-related findings in term of acuity and speed of reading should be incorporated in clinical practice to enhance reading assessment among healthy eyes population.

KEYWORDS: reading acuity; critical print size; reading speed; maximum reading speed; age effect

DOI:10.18240/ijo.2019.07.19

Citation: Chen AH, Khalid NM, Buari NH. Age factor affects reading acuity and reading speed in attaining text information. Int J Ophthalmol 2019;12(7):1170-1176

INTRODUCTION

Reading is a complex skill and earned by learning[1, 2]. As economic changes revolution from traditional industry to information technology era, fluency of reading plays an important role in most educational, occupational and recreational activities[3, 4, 5, 6]. The efficiency to extract text information from our environment is a decisive factor for success in a knowledge-driven, highly-literate society[3, 4, 5, 6]. Our networked digital culture has transformed the way we read[3, 4, 5, 6].

Reading affects the quality of life from childhood to adulthood[7]. Quality of life declines for anyone who experiences difficulty in reading. Reading activity can be affected by several factors such as font size[8], font type[9], field size[10], contrast[11], eye movement[12], and age[13]. In the clinical setting, reading performance could be measured using several parameters such as reading error[14, 15], reading comprehension[16], reading rate[17], reading acuity[18], and reading speed[19]. Two good indicators about the ability of reading are reading acuity and reading speed. Further calculation for acuity and speed, which were the critical print size and maximum reading speed respectively, were the important functional measures of reading performance[19]. It echoes how we read and what we read. The reading speed increased with age, corresponding to 13% words per minute (wpm) from grades 3 to grades 8 of schoolchildren, as well as the improvement of reading acuity and critical print size[2]. The critical print size for children was adult-like by the age of 7 and reading acuity by the age of 9[20]. Healthy aging eyes exhibit subtle visual deficits. After 45 years of age, there are measurably losses especially in the middle and high spatial frequencies[21]. Eye tracking ability suffers similar subtle change with age[22]. Such losses might adversely affect performance on visual tasks involving fine detail, such as reading very small print. There is also evidence that the low-spatial frequency enhancement of contrast sensitivity due to temporal modulation is diminished in people over 60 years of age[23, 24]. Age-related deficits in extracting information from text composed of very large characters might be anticipated due to images of large texts from signage move across the retina during information extraction process. Reading speed declines for smaller or larger characters[7]. If contrast sensitivity plays a role in limiting reading speed, then age-related changes in low and high-spatial-frequency contrast sensitivity will result in reading deficits for large and small characters, respectively. Visual changes in old age other than contrast sensitivity might cause a different pattern of results. Smaller pupil size and increasing density of the crystalline lens both contribute to lower retinal illuminance and might reduce reading speeds at all character sizes. Physiological changes such as reduction of crystalline lens flexibility caused a reduction in quality of near vision that might affect reading performance[25, 26, 27]. Reading rate in normal vision is little affected by large changes in photopic luminance[28]. There might also be non-visual factors affecting reading performance in the elderly. The deterioration in reading performance was also associated with the increasing degree of addition required to see the end point, N8[29]. Reading acuity became the limiting factor[17]. Different types of optotypes, reading acuity and critical print size were essential for near visual function assessment in presbyopes because they were significantly correlated to each other[27]. Older subjects were reported to experience more noticeable deficits, even though they read about the same rate as the young subject when the text was of optimal size[7].

Conventional measures of visual functions, such as visual acuity, had limited value to reflect the performance of the daily task[1]. Skills in reading had gradually been incorporated as a standard measure of functional vision[30]. The reading chart was preferable as reading performance assessment tool, compared to near acuity chart. The full arrangement of words in the reading chart was relatively closer to the real scenario compared to the near letter acuity chart[31]. The baseline data for the reading acuity and reading speed among normally sighted from childhood to old age had been reported previously using MNREAD acuity chart. The study was accumulated over 22y with several incongruent studies, different examiners and different testing locations[13]. The data gathered from numerous studies contained more variation.

Thus, this study was conducted to investigate the effect of age on reading acuity and reading speed in attaining text information in healthy eyes, by minimizing the variation and maintain the consistency using the same examiner and the same testing location. 

SUBJECTS AND METHODS

Ethical Approval  The cross-sectional experimental study design was used to investigate the age effect on reading performance. The study adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the University [Approval Code: 600-RMI ( 5/1/6) REC/108/15]. Informed consent was obtained.

A total of 160 subjects were recruited and grouped according to their age; children (8 to 12 years old), teenagers (13 to 19 years old), young adults (20 to 39 years old), and adults (40 years old and above). Each group comprised of 40 subjects. All subjects were native Malay speakers and fluent in reading and speaking in the Malay language. All subjects had completely healthy eyes (no identifiable pathology). None of the subjects participated had the history of reading problems, visual or cognitive impairment. Subjects were screened for binocular vision procedures to exclude subjects with any latent binocular vision anomalies. Full refraction was carried out prior to the study to achieve an optimum near prescription for presbyopic subjects.

Despite many existing standardized reading charts such as MNREAD acuity chart[32], Bailey-Lovie reading chart[33], Radner Reading Chart[34], and Practical Near Acuity Chart[35], Buari-Chen Malay Reading Chart (BCMRC)[36] was used in our study due to the importance of using language that was familiar and mother tongue to our subjects. It has also been tested to have a good precision (repeatability and reproducibility) to be used as reading research tool and clinical diagnostic tool[37]. Another reason for BCMRC selection was due to the mixed sentence structure of the chart, including contextual sentences (CS) set and random words (RW) set[36]. CS or known as meaningful texts were found to have faster reading speed compared to RW or unrelated texts[38]. Both the reading strategy and the level of comprehension used for the two reading tasks were different[38]. In addition, CS also allowed the subjects to give a correct interpretation of the whole sentences without being conscious of having seen the words in the sentence but not for unrelated words[39]. Moreover, the CS make the task of establishing a near acuity threshold easier, more practical and relevant[36]. The strong correlation of near acuity was found previously between PNAC (CS) and Bailey-Lovie Word Reading Chart (RW)[36]. However, reading the PNAC recorded only half of the time taken by the Bailey-Lovie. Reading the RW took about double the time of reading CS[39]

Reading assessments were conducted in a standardized experimental reading set up. The BCMRC was placed on a reading stand inclined at 45¡ã with the chart illumination range from 250 to 320 lx using a table lamp. Subjects were required to read both sets of BCMRC in random order at a testing distance of 40 cm. The viewing distance was repeatedly measured throughout the reading procedure to maintain the distance. Subjects were instructed to read the chart loudly as quickly and accurately as possible from the largest sentence until the smallest line they could read. Each sentence in the chart was covered with a white matte card to prevent pre-reading. Subjects were required to open each sentence one at a time. The examiner used a voice recorder to record the whole session for analysis purpose.

Reading acuity was defined as the smallest print size that can be read. Reading acuity in our study was calculated using an equation of 1.4¨C(sentence¡Á0.1)+(errors¡Á0.017), where the sentence was a logMAR record for the smallest line a subject read and errors were counted as the number of misread word following the oral reading error categories[14, 15]. The critical print size was the optimal print size for reading or the smallest print that subject could be read with maximum rate[19]. Reading speed and maximum reading speed were measured. Reading speed was determined for each sentence by dividing the number of correct words in the sentence to the time taken (in a minute) to read the sentence[40]. Final reading speed for each subject was the average of reading speed for all print sizes, from the largest print to the reading acuity. An objective measure of the best reading performance could be predicted by calculating maximum reading speed[19]. Normally sighted subjects and most of the low vision subjects will obtain a typical shape of the graph of reading speed versus print size[13]. Reading speed was relatively constant across a large print size, resulted in plateau shape of the graph until it reached one point (critical print size) where the reading speed was significantly dropped. The maximum reading speed was measured by the mean of reading speed from the largest print up to critical print size sentence[41].

The data obtained was then analyzed using the Statistical Package of Social Sciences (SPSS) version 20.0 (USA). The descriptive data were presented as the mean¡ÀSD. A one-way multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was run to determine the effect of different age groups (children, teenagers, young adults, and adults) on reading acuity and reading speed in different sets of sentence structured reading chart; CS set and RW set. The significant level was set at 0.05.

RESULTS

Age of subjects ranged from 8 to 71 years old (mean age: 25.63¡À18.60y). Subjects were classified into four groups; children (9.93¡À1.37y), teenagers (14.88¡À1.44y), young adults (23.08¡À2.57y), and adults (56.25¡À8.30y). Data were normally distributed for each group, as assessed by the Shapiro-Wilk test (P>0.05). One-way MANOVA was conducted to determine if acuity and speed of reading were different between groups with different age in reading both sets of the reading chart. All parameters improved with increasing age.

Reading Acuity  The effect of age on the reading acuity was tabulated in Table 1 and plotted in Figure 1. Data were presented as a mean¡ÀSD. The reading acuity showed fairly steep improvement (reduction of logMAR) from children to teenagers, to young adults, but constant decline (increment of logMAR) towards adults group.

Table 1 Summary of mean and SD for reading acuity and critical print size in four different age groups¡¡¡¡¡¡logMAR

Groups

Reading acuity

Critical print size

CS set

RW set

CS set

RW set

Children

0.07¡À0.08

0.11¡À0.06

0.28¡À0.12

0.31¡À0.11

Teenagers

0.02¡À0.05

0.07¡À0.06

0.15¡À0.12

0.23¡À0.14

Young adults

0.01¡À0.03

0.05¡À0.04

0.11¡À0.10

0.22¡À0.13

Adults

0.08¡À0.08

0.13¡À0.08

0.20¡À0.11

0.25¡À0.11

CS: Contextual sentences; RW: Random words.

Figure 1 Comparison of reading acuity (A) and critical print size (B) among four different age groups in CS set and RW set.

One-way MANOVA was run to determine the effect of age on reading acuity and critical print size. Two sets of reading chart were used, CS set and RW set. All subjects achieved a better acuity (smaller logMAR) in reading CS set compared to RW set. The differences between age group on the reading acuity [F(6, 310)=6.919, P<0.001; Wilks¡¯ ¦«=0.778; partial ¦Ç2=0.118] and critical print size [F(6, 310)=8.268, P<0.001; Wilks¡¯ ¦«=0.743; partial ¦Ç2=0.138] were both statistically significant. Tukey HSD post-hoc tests showed that reading acuity was statistically significant improved from children to teenagers (P<0.05) for both CS set (mean difference =0.05 logMAR; 95%CI=0.01 to 0.08 logMAR) and RW set (mean difference=0.04 logMAR; 95%CI=0.00 to 0.07 logMAR), but significantly deteriorated from young adults to adults (P<0.05) also in both CS set (mean difference=-0.06 logMAR; 95%CI=-0.10 to -0.03 logMAR) and RW set (mean difference=-0.08 logMAR; 95%CI=-0.11 to -0.04 logMAR). No significant difference in reading acuity between teenagers and young adults and also between children and adults (P>0.05). For critical print size, Tukey HSD post-hoc revealed different outcomes between CS set and RW set. CS set had statistically improved (P<0.05) critical print size from children to teenagers (mean difference=0.14 logMAR; 95%CI=0.07 to 0.20 logMAR) and between children and adults (mean difference=0.09 logMAR; 95%CI=0.02 to 0.15 logMAR). Critical print size also significantly declined (P<0.05) from young adults to adults (mean difference=-0.09 logMAR; 95%CI=-0.15 to -0.02 logMAR) for CS set. RW set only showed significant improved (P<0.05) critical print size between children to teenagers (mean difference=0.07 logMAR; 95%CI=0.00 to 0.15 logMAR), but no significant difference found between other groups comparison (P>0.05).

Reading Speed  The speed of reading was quantified using two parameters of reading, reading speed and maximum reading speed. Data were presented as the mean¡ÀSD tabulated in Table 2 and illustrated in Figure 2. According to age order, the reading speed increased from children to teenagers, about plateau from teenagers to young adults, and decreased in reaching adults. The effect of age on the reading speed was analyzed using one-way MANOVA. All subjects read faster in reading CS set compared to RW set. The differences between age group on the reading speed was statistically significant [F(6, 310)=13.887, P<0.001; Wilks¡¯ ¦«=0.621; partial ¦Ç2=0.212], as well as comparison of maximum reading speed among different age [F(6, 310)=14.149, P<0.001; Wilks¡¯ ¦«=0.616; partial ¦Ç2=0.215].

Table 2 Summary of mean and SD for reading speed and maximum reading speed in four different age groups

Groups

Reading speed (wpm)

Maximum reading speed (wpm)

CS set

RW set

CS set

RW set

Children

144.25¡À29.17

98.09¡À21.95

159.40¡À30.49

105.38¡À22.94

Teenagers

190.46¡À30.58

140.15¡À24.41

198.77¡À28.20

148.76¡À24.03

Young adults

193.12¡À25.58

137.59¡À19.98

199.71¡À25.98

149.06¡À20.35

Adults

164.54¡À27.63

116.15¡À27.35

177.45¡À23.57

122.75¡À27.54

CS: Contextual sentences; RW: Random words.

Figure 2 Comparison of reading speed (A) and maximum reading speed (B) among four different age groups in CS set and RW set.

Tukey HSD post hoc analysis revealed that the increment of reading speed from children to teenagers (CS set: MD=-46.20 wpm; 95%CI=-62.64 to -29.77 wpm, RW set: MD=-42.06 wpm; 95%CI=-55.75 to -28.37 wpm) and also decrement of reading speed from young adults to adults (CS set: MD=28.58 wpm; 95%CI=12.15 to 45.02 wpm, RW set: MD=21.44 wpm; 95%CI=7.75 to 35.14 wpm) were statistically significant (P<0.05), about 14% and 9% respectively, but no significant difference of reading speed found between teenagers and young adults (P>0.05). Similar to reading speed, maximum reading speed also increased from children, to teenagers, to young adults, but decreased in adults, as according to age order. About 14% increment (CS set: MD=-39.38 wpm; 95%CI= -55.16 to -23.59 wpm, RW set: MD=-43.38 wpm; 95%CI= -57.23 to -29.53 wpm) of maximum reading speed from children to teenagers and 8% decrement (CS set: MD=22.26 wpm; 95%CI=6.47 to 38.04 wpm, RW set: MD=26.31 wpm; 95%CI=12.46 to 40.17 wpm) from young adults to adults were proven to be significantly different (P<0.05) by Tukey HSD post hoc analysis. Summary of reading speed and maximum reading speed were tabulated in Table 2 and plotted in Figure 2.

DISCUSSION

Reading Acuity  There was a steady increment in reading acuity and critical print size from children to teenagers and young adults before they declined in adults. The previous study has shown that a steep improvement pattern of reading acuity from children (-0.10 logMAR at the age of 8) to teenage years (-0.18 logMAR at the age of 16) and declined throughout adulthood (-0.05 logMAR by age of 81)[13]. Our study found similar pattern, where the reading acuity improved from children (CS set: 0.07 logMAR; RW set: 0.11 logMAR) to teenagers (CS set: 0.02 logMAR; RW set: 0.07 logMAR), to young adults (CS set: 0.01 logMAR; RW set: 0.05 logMAR), and declined in adults group (CS set: 0.08 logMAR; RW set: 0.13 logMAR). Critical print size in our study also displayed a similar pattern as per in reading acuity. However, it was contradicted to the previous claim that stated a different pattern in which critical print size was a plateau for children and teenagers, the slight increment in logMAR for young and middle-aged, and pronounced increase for older population[13]. This contradiction might be due to the dissimilarity of age range distribution among children. Children in previous study aged from 8 to 16 years old[13], while children in our study aged from 8 to 12 years old. Even though the visual acuity for children was found to be fully matured at the age of 5 years[42], but it might not reflect the reading abilities and skills.

Our findings for reading acuity using Buari-Chen Malay Reading Chart (BCMRC) was found higher in logMAR compared to previous studies findings using other standard reading charts such as MNREAD acuity charts and Radner Reading Chart (RRC). This might be due to the different limit of print sizes. Print size is closely linked to spatial frequency. Previous studies examined the age effect on the spatial frequency and reading rate might shield some clues on this hypothetical assumption. In a study where reading rate was measured as a function of angular character size for groups of young (mean age 21.6y) and old (mean age 68.3y) subjects[7], the old group showed greater deficits for large (12 degrees) and small (0.15 degree) characters than for characters of intermediate size. They argued based on the fundamental characteristic of the spatial frequency in the font size. Reading rates for normally sighted subjects were reported to be the greatest for a range of intermediate character sizes ranging from about 0.3 degrees to 2 degrees[17]. The spatial-frequency resolution required for reading 0.15 degree characters is about 13 cycles per degree (cpd)[43]. The fundamental spatial-frequency for a 0.15 degree character size is approximately 6.7 cpd. Reading requires spatial frequencies extending to at least twice the fundamental frequency. Contrast sensitivity of 13 cpd in old age was also reported to reduce by about a factor of 3 from young normal values[24, 43, 44]. At this character size, the old group reads at 66% of the rate of the young group. Therefore, losses in contrast sensitivity can account quantitatively for the age-related reduction in reading rate at 0.15 degree. MNREAD acuity chart consisted of a range of print sizes until -0.5 logMAR[40] and RRC had print sizes until -0.2 logMAR[45], while BCMRC had the smallest print size of 0.00 logMAR[36], similar with Bailey-Lovie chart[34] and Practical Near Acuity Chart[36]. The threshold for reading acuity was 0.00 logMAR which was equivalent to 6/6 or 20/ 20 in Snellen notation. The print size displayed above the threshold level like MNREAD acuity chart and RRC might give advantages especially in assessing acuity among normal vision population but also might induce demoralization among the visually impaired population as there could be many sentences left that cannot be read.

Reading Speed  Both reading speed and maximum reading speed increased with age and deteriorated when reached adults age, 40y and above. Our findings on the reading speed for children were aligned with the previous report using MNREAD acuity chart. They reported that maximum reading speed increased by 65 wpm from 8 to 15 years old, about plateau for young adults with slight decrement by only 1.68 wpm (16 to 40 years old), and reduced for older population by 25 wpm (from 40 to 81 years old)[13]. The increment of reading speed in our study also showed drastic changes from children (mean age: 9.93y) to teenagers (mean age: 14.88y) by approximately 42.76 wpm. It might due to the maturation of the white matter in the left temporal-parietal of brain connectivity that correlated to the range of reading ability among children 8 to 12 years old[46]. The mean reading speed among children was found to be lower in the MNREAD acuity chart compared to our findings of the reading speed on CS set, 126.18 wpm[2]. It might due to the method used for reading which was monocular, each eye for each chart[2]. However, binocularity was found to be physiologically correlated to reduce fixation disparity during fixation, and minimum amount of fixation disparity reflected the optimal binocular status[47]. The mean data of maximum reading speed for Radner Reading Chart among children[48] was found to be slightly higher (169.46 wpm), compared to maximum reading speed for CS set in our study (159.40 wpm). The age range was different. Their age range was slightly older (10 to 12y), compared to our study (8 to 12y). Different age represented different grade in school. One grade indicated one additional year of education. That made a difference because reading speed increased with school grade[2].

Our study confirmed that no increment from teenagers to young adults which was in agreement with the previous study[13]. The reading speed remained almost constant from 16 to 40 years old, as the reading skills had fully developed[13]. The range of reading speed among teenagers and young adults in our study supported previous studies that assessed the reading speed among the same age group[42, 45, 49, 50, 51]. The reading speed then decreased later in life. As found in our study, the reading speed among adults was significantly reduced compared to young adults, about 9% decrement in wpm. The reduction of reading speed among adults supported the previous study that reported the maximum reading speed decreased from 200 wpm in young adults to 175 wpm in adults[13]. Reading ability was found significantly declined with age even among a group of people with good acuity[52]. Three possible contributing factors for the apparent reduction in reading speed among older adults: reduced contrast sensitivity in increasing age[53], deterioration in motor processing such as eye movement[54], and defective transient system[7]. Contrast sensitivity reduced by about a factor of 3 for older subjects[23]. Oculomotor limitations provided an alternative qualitative explanation for the age-related reading speed reduction. There was evidence of age-related deficits in eye-movement control[22]. A defective transient system in older subjects with longer lasting neural images at low-spatial frequency might affect the reading speed[55, 56]. Other factors affecting reading in older age including slowing down in visual processing[55] and declined in cognitive processing[57].

In conclusion, reading performance equivalent to age among healthy eyes population was established using the Buari-Chen Malay Reading Chart. The information could be a useful guide in assessing reading acuity and reading speed in clinical practice. Limitation of our study was the lacking of normative data for different types of visual impairment. Future research might focus on developing norms for different types of visual impairments for visual rehabilitation purposes.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Foundation: Supported by the Research Grant Scheme 600-IRMI-DANA 5/3/BESTARI (00013/2016) and 600-IRMI/MYRA 5/3/MITRA (007/2017)-2 from the Universiti Teknologi MARA, Malaysia

Conflicts of Interest: Chen AH, None; Khalid NM, None; Buari NH, None.

REFERENCES

1 Legge GE, Ross JA, Isenberg LM, LaMay JM. Psychophysics of reading. Clinical predictors of low-vision reading speed. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 1992;33(3):677-687.

 

2 Virgili G, Cordaro C, Bigoni A, Crovato S, Cecchini P, Menchini U. Reading acuity in children: evaluation and reliability using MNREAD charts. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2004;45(9):3349-3354.
https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.03-1304
PMid:15326160

 

3 Meagan L. The Slow Book Revolution: Creating a New Culture of Reading on College Campuses and Beyond. California: ABC-CLIO, 2014.

 

4 Castells M. The Information Age, Volumes 1-3: Economy, Society and Culture. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell, 1999.

 

5 Gillings MR, Hilbert M, Kemp DJ. Information in the biosphere: biological and digital worlds. Trends Ecol Evol (Amst) 2016;31(3):180-189.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2015.12.013
PMid:26777788

 

6 McGowan R. The work of nations: Preparing ourselves for the 21st century capitalism, by Robert Reich. New York: Knopf Publishing, 1991. Hum Resour Manage 1991;30(4):535-538.
https://doi.org/10.1002/hrm.3930300407

 

7 Akutsu H, Legge GE, Ross JA, Schuebel KJ. Psychophysics of reading: X. Effects of age-related changes in vision. J Gerontol 1991;46(6): P325-P331.
https://doi.org/10.1093/geronj/46.6.P325

 

8 Chung ST. Size or spacing: which limits letter recognition in people with age-related macular degeneration? Vision Res 2014; 101:167-176.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2014.06.015
PMid:25014400 PMCid:PMC4169903

 

9 Kaspar K, Wehlitz T, von Knobelsdorff S, Wulf T, von Saldern MA. A matter of font type: The effect of serifs on the evaluation of scientific abstracts. Int J Psychol 2015;50(5):372-378.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12160
PMid:25704872

 

10 Mathews PM, Rubin GS, McCloskey M, Salek S, Ramulu PY. Severity of vision loss interacts with word-specific features to impact out-loud reading in glaucoma. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2015;56(3): 1537-1545.
https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.15-15462
PMid:25737150 PMCid:PMC4349108

 

11 Brussee T, van den Berg TJTP, van Nispen RMA, de Boer I, van Rens GHMB. Association between contrast sensitivity and reading with macular pathology. Optom Vis Sci 2018;95(3):183-192.
https://doi.org/10.1097/OPX.0000000000001178
PMid:29401182

 

12 Burton R, Smith ND, Crabb DP. Eye movements and reading in glaucoma: observations on patients with advanced visual field loss. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol 2014;252(10):1621-1630.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00417-014-2752-x
PMid:25074043

 

13 Calabr¨¨se A, Cheong AM, Cheung SH, He Y, Kwon M, Mansfield JS, Subramanian A, Yu D, Legge GE. Baseline MNREAD measures for normally sighted subjects from childhood to old age. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2016;57(8):3836-3843.
https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.16-19580
PMid:27442222 PMCid:PMC4961000

 

14 Douglas G, Grimley M, Hill E, Long R, Tobin M. The use of the NARA for assessing the reading ability of children with low vision. Br J Vis Impair 2002;20(2):68-75.
https://doi.org/10.1177/026461960202000204

 

15 Khalid NM, Buari NH, Chen AH. Comparison of oral reading errors between contextual sentences and random words among schoolchildren. IES 2016;10(1):47.
https://doi.org/10.5539/ies.v10n1p47

 

16 Savolainen H, Ahonen T, Aro M, Tolvanen A, Holopainen L. Reading comprehension, word reading and spelling as predictors of school achievement and choice of secondary education. Learn Instr 2008;18(2):201-210.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.09.017

 

17 Legge GE, Pelli DG, Rubin GS, Schleske MM. Psychophysics of reading: I. normal vision. Vision Res 1985;25(2):239-252.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(85)90117-8

 

18 Colenbrander A. Reading acuity: an important parameter of reading performance. Int Congr Ser 2005;1282:487-491.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ics.2005.05.003

 

19 Mansfield JS, Legge GE, Bane MC. Psychophysics of reading. XV: Font effects in normal and low vision. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 1996;37(8):1492-1501.

 

20 Braswell J, Rine RM. Evidence that vestibular hypofunction affects reading acuity in children. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 2006;70(11): 1957-1965.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2006.07.013
PMid:16945429

 

21 Sieiro RDO, Coelho LM, Boas PCV, Fonseca SC, Souza SR, Guimarães TDP. Contrast sensitivity assessment in different age group in medium and high spatial frequency. Rev Bras Oftalmol 2016;75(4):296-299.
https://doi.org/10.5935/0034-7280.20160059

 

22 Seferlis F, Chimona TS, Papadakis CE, Bizakis J, Triaridis S, Skoulakis C. Age related changes in ocular motor testing in healthy subjects. J Vestib Res 2015;25(2):57-66.

 

23 Sloane ME, Owsley C, Jackson CA. Aging and luminance-adaptation effects on spatial contrast sensitivity. J Opt Soc Am A 1988; 5(12):2181-2190.
https://doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.5.002181

 

24 Wright CE, Drasdo N. The influence of age on the spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity function. Doc Ophthalmol 1985;59(4):385-395.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00159172
PMid:4028926

 

25 Patel I, Munoz B, Burke AG, Kayongoya A, McHiwa W, Schwarzwalder AW, West SK. Impact of presbyopia on quality of life in a rural African setting. Ophthalmology 2006;113(5):728-734.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ophtha.2006.01.028
PMid:16650665

 

26 Patel I, West SK. Presbyopia: prevalence, impact, and interventions. Community Eye Health 2007;20(63):40-41.

 

27 Gupta N, Wolffsohn JS, Naroo SA. Comparison of near visual acuity and reading metrics in presbyopia correction. J Cataract Refract Surg 2009;35(8):1401-1409.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrs.2009.03.026
PMid:19631128

 

28 Legge GE, Rubin GS. Psychophysics of reading. IV. Wavelength effects in normal and low vision. J Opt Soc Am A 1986;3(1):40-51.
https://doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.3.000040
PMid:3950791

 

29 Sherwin JC, Keeffe JE, Kuper H, Islam FM, Muller A, Mathenge W. Functional presbyopia in a rural Kenyan population: the unmet presbyopic need. Clin Exp Ophthalmol 2008;36(3):245-251.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1442-9071.2008.01711.x
PMid:18412593

 

30 Legge GE, Klitz TS, Tjan BS. Mr. Chips: an ideal-observer model of reading. Psychol Rev 1997;104(3):524-553.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.104.3.524

 

31 Oduntan AO, al-Abdulmunem MA. Design of an Arabic near visual acuity chart. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 1997;17(2):158-160.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-1313.1997.tb00035.x

 

32 Mansfield S, Ahn SJ, Legge GE, Luebker A. A new reading acuity chart for normal and low vision. Ophtalmic Vis Opt 1993;3:232-235.

 

33 Bailey IL, Lovie JE. The design and use of a new near-vision chart. Am J Optom Physiol Opt 1980;57(6):378-387.
https://doi.org/10.1097/00006324-198006000-00011
PMid:7406006

 

34 Radner W, Willinger U, Obermayer W, Mudrich C, Velikay-Parel M, Eisenwort B. A new German Reading Chart for the simultaneous evaluation of reading acuity and reading speed. Klin Monbl Augenheilkd 1998;213(9):174-181.
https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2008-1034969
PMid:9793916

 

35 Wolffsohn JS, Cochrane AL. The practical near acuity chart (PNAC) and prediction of visual ability at near. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2000;20(2):90-97.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0275-5408(99)00035-6

 

36 Buari NH, Chen A-H. Buari-Chen Malay Reading Chart (BCMRC): Contextual Sentence and Random Words 2-in-1 Design in Malay. Pertanika J Sci Techmology 2017;25(S)135-150.

 

37 Khalid NM, Chen AH. Repeatability and Reproducibility of Buari-Chen Malay Reading Chart. Asian J Appl Sci 2018;06(01):1-8.

 

38 Carver RP. Reading Rate: A Review of Research and Theory. Academic Press; 1990. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1990-98853-000.

 

39 Latham K, Whitaker D. A comparison of word recognition and reading performance in foveal and peripheral vision. Vision Res 1996;36(17): 2665-2674.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(96)00022-3

 

40 Subramanian A, Pardhan S. The repeatability of MNREAD acuity charts and variability at different test distances. Optom Vis Sci 2006;83(8):572-576.
https://doi.org/10.1097/01.opx.0000232225.00311.53
PMid:16909082

 

41 Maaijwee K, Mulder P, Radner W, Van Meurs JC. Reliability testing of the Dutch version of the Radner Reading Charts. Optom Vis Sci 2008;85(5):353-358.
https://doi.org/10.1097/OPX.0b013e31816bf58b
PMid:18451739

 

42 Leat SJ, Yadav NK, Irving EL. Development of visual acuity and contrast sensitivity in children. J Optom 2009;2(1):19-26.
https://doi.org/10.3921/joptom.2009.19
PMCid:PMC3972638

 

43 Legge GE, Rubin GS, Luebker A. Psychophysics of reading: V. The role of contrast in normal vision. Vision Res 1987;27(7):1165-1177.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(87)90028-9

 

44 Owsley C, Sekuler R, Siemsen D. Contrast sensitivity throughout adulthood. Vision Res 1983;23(7):689-699.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(83)90210-9

 

45 Radner W, Obermayer W, Richter-Mueksch S, Willinger U, Velikay-Parel M, Eisenwort B. The validity and reliability of short German sentences for measuring reading speed. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol 2002;240(6):461-467.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00417-002-0443-5
PMid:12107513

 

46 Beaulieu C, Plewes C, Paulson LA, Roy D, Snook L, Concha L, Phillips L. Imaging brain connectivity in children with diverse reading ability. Neuroimage 2005;25(4):1266-1271.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.053
PMid:15850744

 

47 Jainta S, Hoormann J, Kloke WB, Jaschinski W. Binocularity during reading fixations: Properties of the minimum fixation disparity. Vision Res 2010;50(18):1775-1785.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010.05.033
PMid:20573592

 

48 Stifter E, Burggasser G, Hirmann E, Thaler A, Radner W. Evaluating reading acuity and speed in children with microstrabismic amblyopia using a standardized reading chart system. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol 2005;243(12):1228-1235.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00417-005-1187-9
PMid:16003512

 

49 Buari NH, Azizan MF, Chen AH. Comparison of reading speed using malay unrelated word reading chart with standardized english reading charts. Int J Med Heal Sci Res 2015;2(3):55-61.
https://doi.org/10.18488/journal.9/2015.2.3/9.3.55.61

 

50 Buari NH, Chen AH, Musa N. Comparison of reading speed with 3 different log-scaled reading charts. J Optom 2014;7(4):210-216.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.optom.2013.12.009
PMid:25323642 PMCid:PMC4213869

 

51 İDİL ŞA, ÇALIŞKAN D, İDİL NB. Development and validation of the Turkish version of the MNREAD visual acuity charts. Turk J Med Sci 2011;41(4):565-570.

 

52 Lott LA, Schneck ME, Haegerström-Portnoy G, Brabyn JA, Gildengorin GL, West CG. Reading performance in older adults with good acuity. Optom Vis Sci 2001;78(5):316-324.
https://doi.org/10.1097/00006324-200105000-00015

 

53 Owsley C. Aging and vision. Vision Res 2011;51(13):1610-1622.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010.10.020
PMid:20974168 PMCid:PMC3049199

 

54 Green HJ, Lemaire P, Dufau S. Eye movement correlates of younger and older adults' strategies for complex addition. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2007;125(3):257-278.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.08.001
PMid:17007804

 

55 Kline DW, Schieber F. Visual aging: a transient/sustained shift. Percept Psychophys 1981;29(2):181-182.
https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03207284
PMid:7255099

 

56 Feng J, Craik FIM, Levine B, Moreno S, Naglie G, Choi H. Differential age-related changes in localizing a target among distractors across an extended visual field. Eur J Ageing 2017;14(2):167-177.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10433-016-0399-7
PMid:28804400 PMCid:PMC5550660

 

57 Paterson KB, McGowan VA, Jordan TR. Effects of adult aging on reading filtered text: evidence from eye movements. Peer J 2013;1:e63.
https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.63
PMid:23646282 PMCid:PMC3642708